(adapted from section 5, "Respiratory, postural and spatio-kinetic motor stabilization, internal models, top-down timed motor coordination and expanded cerebello-cerebral circuitry: a review".)
While the term “dexterity” is
exclusively used of humans, the term
“bipedality” is not. The simple act of positioning the body on two legs
is
widespread amongst terrestrial vertebrates (Alexander 2004). Humans
are,
however, unique amongst extant animals in three respects. Humans use
(i)
a biped bauplan
(literally “building plan”) based upon anatomical vertical alignment
(Borelli
1989/1680, proposition CXXXV),
(ii)
this fully extended
upright posture is strongly robust against vigorous or disruptive
perturbation,
including those that occur when such bipedality is combined with expert
upper
body actions (such as accurate throwing, carrying saucers of easily
spilt
fluids), or when footing is temporarily or potentially lost (during
trips and
fights), and
(iii)
this upright arrangement
is constantly accompanied by an unconscious musculoskeletal “dance” of
anticipatory and feedforward postural adjustments (Belen'kii, Gurfinkel
et al.
1967; Gelfand, Gurfinkel et al. 1971; Gurfinkel, Kots et al. 1971;
Cordo and
Nashner 1982; Bouisset and Zattara 1987; Massion 1992; Hodges,
Gurfinkel et al.
2002).
Here it is argued that it is
the uniquely enhanced capacity of humans for
predictive internal models that by enabling continuous timed
stabilizing
adjustments, that allows humans, and only humans, to uniquely stand and
move
their body carriage in fully extended vertical alignment. Further, and
again
uniquely, that this allows humans to have the capacity to engage with
robust
stability in secondary (and quick center of mass shifting) upper and
lower body
part expert actions. Whether, and far, these traits characterized
Australopiths
is also discussed.
Anatomical
vertical alignment bauplan
The human body is structured
nearly entirely upon the compressive
anatomical arrangement of bones, ligaments, joints and cartilage
support
"pads" (menisci). As a result, the human body lacks flexion between
each of the body carriage segments—head, thorax, thigh, shank—as they
are
stacked in vertical alignment above each other. The ankle and
metatarsophalangeal articulation in the feet are the only joints that
humans
properly flex normally in standing stance, and in the ground contact
stages of
walking. In running, the knee also is flexed but this links to the
separate
factor in which the running leg functions as a shock absorber when it
hits the
ground (Nigg, Bahlsen et al. 1987). Remarkably, this anatomical
vertical
compressive alignment bauplan is not employed, even occasionally, by
any other
extant terrestrial biped. Penguins and meerkats (temporarily) may
appear to be
spinally erect but they balance with a tripod use of their tails, and
their
bodies and legs are, on close examination, in fact flexed. Changes to
foot
anatomy (which reduced its flexion and enhanced its compressive
stability)
suggest the full development of such alignment dates at least to
2.2-2.36 myr
and early Homo (Gebo and Schwartz 2006).
In contrast to humans, all
nonhuman bipeds (birds, extinct dinosaurs and
primates) use a variety of flexion biped bauplan in which the body
carriage
segments are angled relative to each other. In the case of nonhuman
primates
when they occasionally stand or walk bipedally, their limbs are flexed
and
their thorax is bent forward (Okada 1985; Schmitt 2003; Alexander
2004). As a
result, when upright they flex their legs and thorax in a "bent-knee,
bent
hip" or compliant manner (Okada 1985; Schmitt 2003; Alexander 2004).
Even
anatomically aligned ratites such as ostriches limit such alignment to
only one
of their four leg joints (their extended “knee”—actually the joint
between
their tibia and tarso-metatarsus), and this in spite of appearances is
slightly
flexed with its body weight being only partial transmitted
compressively
directly down the joint (personal communication, Jonas Rubenson). Large
≥900 kg
“graviportal” quadruped animals (Gregory 1912) use alignment and
compression
but again this is confined to the legs, and they, of course, have the
benefit
of the stability that comes from a four cornered quadruped “support”.
Instability
and bipedal risk of falls
Borelli (1989/1680) was the
first to note that the anatomical vertical
alignment bauplan is intrinsically unstable and so needs constant
musculoskeletal adjustment if fall and injury are not to occur: “the
erect position
is unstable as a result of the slipperiness of the joints.... They
[humans]
need their muscles to correct displacements and prevent falling”
Borelli (1989/1680:
p. 130) (see also Skoyles submitted).
This lack of stability and the
need for constant postural adjustment
particularly in fast walking and running puts the body at risk of
postural and
locomotive interruption due to falls, and more importantly, creates the
risk of
severe leg injury, and as a result, temporary or even permanent
immobilization.
This is because the kinetic energy held in the upright adult human body
compared to that needed to break a femur bone is “an order of magnitude
greater
than the maximum value of the work to fracture and nearly twenty times
the
average work to fracture (Lotz and Hayes 1990: p. 698). In addition to
this, as
humans get taller in stature, the strength of their long support bones
fails to
keep up in their osteological strength with the forces that impact upon
them
from standing height falls. A child can fall, for example, with only a
bruise
but an adult can suffer a substantial injury. (The effect is marked and
nonlinear: osteological strength decreases at
the square root
relative to increased height; for a review of the riskiness of
bipedally see
Skoyles submitted).
Stability and
engineering
The flexion biped bauplan, in
contrast to anatomically aligned
bipedality, provides opportunities for low-level self-stabilizing. When
upright, putting support limbs or the body at an angle allows that
shifts of
the body’s center of mass over its support base can be corrected by
changing
slightly that angle. Even simple spring arrangements such as found in
articulated (anglepoise) lamps can be arranged to do this automatically
(French
and Widden 2000). Not only does flexion allow this greater opportunity
for
quick equilibrium management, but flexed limbs and thorax, can be
further
initially angled such that preflexes and spinal adjustment reflexes are
to
optimal for aiding this automatic adjustment. This option to stabilize
the
upright body, however, does not exist if limbs and the thorax are not
angled.
Instead, the extended erect body to maintain its centre of mass over
its feet
must actively and constantly shift masses distant to the legs including
changing the posture of the spine and thorax (Hodges, Gurfinkel et al.
2002).
The need for such changes due to the precariousness of human unflexed
uprightness is illustrated by the fact that they are even made to
correct the
shifts in the centre of mass projection created by respiration
(Gurfinkel, Kots
et al. 1971; Hodges, Gurfinkel et al. 2002).
Utility of
the human bauplan
The anatomical vertical
alignment bauplan of humans would not have arisen
without advantages that compensate for its intrinsic instability and
unsafeness. These are significant. Notably, humans can economically
stand erect
for long periods since they use only 7% more energy standing than when
laying
down; in contrast a 10 kg dog when quadrupedally standing uses 70% more
than
when supine (Abitbol 1988). This is because anatomical alignment does
not
require energy expenditure to maintain muscles in a state of flexion. A
further
utility of the anatomically aligned bauplan is that when walking, the
stiff
compressive axis through the upright body allows it to temporarily
store
potential energy (in the form of a raised center of mass at midstance)
from
forward kinetic energy (such locomotion is also known as strident or
inverted
pendulum walking) (Cavagna, Thys et al. 1976). In such strident
walking, the
stiff leg temporarily vaults up the mass of the body and so stores
potential
energy that can be reused to swing forth the leg to make the next step.
In
contrast, bent knee, bent hip walking used by other primates in their
occasional bipedalism requires about four times the energy due to the
need in
addition to moving the body forward of maintaining flexed muscles in a
constant
state of tension (Sockol, Raichlen et al. 2007). There are also likely
to be
other advantages linked to enhanced maneuverability of the upper body
(Skoyles,
in preparation).
The existence of these
advantages raises the problem of why evolution has
only exploited them in one extant primate. Given the instability of
bipedal
anatomical alignment, this suggests that evolution has only found in
the
particular case of Homo, an effective means to
balance constantly such
an intrinsically unsteady and difficult to stabilize anatomical aligned
posture, particularly when engaging in complex forms of bipedality such
as
rough ground running and doing upper body actions (such as carrying and
throwing). It is suggested here that this was linked to brain
expansion, and
the consequently increased ability to constantly engage in top-down
anticipatory postural adjustments.
Australopithecine
bipedality
Considerable controversy exists
over the nature of Australopithecine
terrestrial bipedality as to whether it was stiff gait (and so
anatomically
aligned) (Latimer 1991; Ohman, Krochta et al. 1997; Lovejoy 2006), or
compliant
(Susman, Stern et al. 1984; Stern 2000). Evidence arguing for
anatomical
alignment and stiff gait are musculoskeletal features such as femoral
bicondylar angle, and the increase in total cancellous bone in primary
joints
of lower limbs (Latimer 2005). Another factor is the biomechanical
modeling of
the efficiency of hominin walking (Crompton, Yu et al. 1998). A further
factor
is that macaques after initial training in bipedal erect standing can
be
trained successfully in semi-anatomical aligned strident walking
(Hirasakia,
Ogiharab et al. 2004). This argues that the primate engagement in stiff
gait
walking is only to a limited extent related to lack of anatomical
adaptation,
otherwise it could not be acquired by behavioral modification as in
macaques.
This suggests other nonanatomical factors (such as ecological utility)
are
responsible for its hominin adoption, with anatomical changes being
secondary
to these behavioral ones.
The above listed instability
factors are greatest for fast walking and
running, that are specific to Homo. Foot anatomy of
preHomo (unlike
Homo) suggests that their feet (Berillon 2004; Gebo
and Schwartz 2006)
were not optimal for running, as was the lack of the lateral
stabilization
provided by a Homo-like gluteus maximus,
(Lieberman, Raichlen et al.
2006). Bramble and Lieberman (2004) have identified diverse
musculoskeletal
adaptations needed for endurance running in Homo
that are absent in preHomo
hominins. Thus, the stabilization problem faced by Homo
is much more
severe than that faced by Australopiths. Since the balancing for slow
walking
can be acquired by modern trained bipedal macaques (Hirasakia, Ogiharab
et al.
2004)), this level of postural stabilization is likely to be within the
already
existing postural capacities of preHomo brains.
Further, Australopiths were
less at risk from falls than Homo.
These habitus factors (that
Australopiths share with other apes) have
been argued elsewhere (submitted) relate to the need to minimize the
injury
risk of arboreal falls. However, they would have also as a side
consequence
allowed Australopiths to be bipedal without also needing an highly
developed
capacity to stabilize the upright body. In contrast, Homo
individuals
when running without the protective habitus of earlier hominins and
arboreal
apes would place themselves at constant risk, given the body’s
momentum, of
immobilization if they slipped or tripped injuring a lower limb.
Preventing
this requires considerable motor skill not only in controlling body
falls in
such circumstances, but also the ability to integrate into locomotion
visual
and other information about the rough ground ahead to constantly make
safe
footings.
How might we understand
Australopith bipedality? An important factor here
is that human bipedality even in simple walking and standing radically
changes
after childhood as internal model competence expands, in terms of motor
qualities such as greater postural robustness (Hirschfeld and Forssberg
1992),
and stabilization of the head and vision (Assaiante and Amblard 1995).
This
changes involves a reorganization in how the erect body is controlled
(Assaiante
and Amblard 1995). An adult flexibly adapts their balance mobility
across their
head, upper arms, lower arms, hands, thorax and any held objects.
However, they
are "locked together" in children as they lack the sophisticated
adult ability needed to stabilize and control all their separate
degrees of
freedom as a single kinematic mobility (Assaiante and Amblard 1995).
Early Homo
show minor anatomical changes such as to the distal femur at
adolescence (Tardieu
1998) that suggest this change to a more sophisticated stabilization of
balance
only occurred in Homo evolution. The more simple
"locked
together" anatomical alignment found in modern children not present
before
Homo adolescence would have been adequate for the
more limited
locomotion needs of Australopiths. Thus, while anatomical aligned
bipedality
may be unique to hominins, the combination of this with highly
developed
internal model based postural adjustment might be a developmental
limited to
adult and adolescent Homo.
Vertical
alignment bauplan, internal models
and cerebello-cerebral circuits
Research since the 1960s shows
that humans (as Borelli suggested three
centuries earlier) stabilize their upright bodies by continuous
musculoskeletal
adjustment (Belen'kii, Gurfinkel et al. 1967; Gelfand, Gurfinkel et al.
1971;
Gurfinkel, Kots et al. 1971; Cordo and Nashner 1982; Bouisset and
Zattara 1987;
Massion 1992; Hodges, Gurfinkel et al. 2002). In this, motor commands
that will
cause a forward displacement of body mass such as moving an arm forward
to
reach an object, are accompanied by top-down timed anticipatory
postural
adjustments that ensure that it is accompanied by an equal and opposite
counter-positioning of body mass (Cordo and Nashner 1982). Not only do
such
adjustments stabilize the body but such timed postural submovements
accompanying walking steps help to propel them and make locomotion more
steady
and efficient (MacKinnon, Bissig et al. 2007). Feedforward processes
incorporating sensory inputs into internal models (Desmurget and
Grafton 2000)
are also likely to be particularly important, where anticipation is
limited
such when making stepping reactions if
erectness is threatened (Zettel, McIlroy et al.
2002; Marigold, Bethune et al. 2003; Hughey and Fung 2005; Pai, Yang et
al.
2006); and the organized control of body collapse so it falls in a
manner that
minimises injury (Hsiao and Robinovitch 1998).
Consistent with this dependence
of human bipedality upon continuous timed
postural adjustment, there is considerable evidence that normal human
bipedality is supraspinal and involves the cerebellum working with the
cerebral
cortex. Dual-tasks, for example, show that postural adjustment requires
cognitive resources used by other motor faculties such as vocal
articulators
(repeating syllables, talking) (Lundin-Olsson, Nyberg et al. 1997;
Yardley,
Gardner et al. 2000; de Hoon, Allum et al. 2003; VanderVelde,
Woollacott et al.
2005), dexterity (Ebersbach, Dimitrijevic et al. 1995), and even higher
cognitions such as spatial tasks, mathematics and Stroop (Woollacott
and
Shumway-Cook 2002; Hauer, Pfisterer et al. 2003; VanderVelde,
Woollacott et al.
2005; Maki and McIlroy 2007). The close study of maintaining stance and
walking
shows that it is stabilized by central control (Morasso and Schieppati
1999),
and involves cerebello-cerebral cortex circuits (Jacobs and Horak 2007;
Maki
and McIlroy 2007). It can include—if balance is particularly
challenging—also
the involvement of the prefrontal cortex, for example, when people walk
on
treadmills (Suzuki, Miyai et al. 2004), or if a person stands in a
virtual
reality that lacks visual clues as to the floor and the ceiling
(Slobounov, Wu
et al. 2006). People can motor empathize with the postural upright
stability in
other individuals (Slobounov, Tutwiler et al. 2000; Cheng, Tzeng et al.
2005),
and this occurs through cortical mirror neurons (Cheng, Tzeng et al.
2005).
This is all suggestive or consistent with a dependence upon postural
adjustment
based upon cerebello-cerebral cortex predictive internal models.
Noninternal
model bipedal alternatives
Anticipatory postural
adjustments and such internal model based top-down
stabilization, it needs to be noted, is not required, however, for all
forms of
bipedal stiff standing and walking. Mechanical toys and robots can be
designed
such that their passive mechanical properties can cause them to walk in
a
human-like stiff manner on flat surfaces (Fallis 1888; Collins, Ruina
et al.
2005). Empirical research suggests that stable upright posture can be
achieved
with only an open loop ankle stiffness control (Winter, Patla et al.
1998), or
a closed loop involving muscle spindle and monosynaptic spinal feedback
mechanisms (He, Levine et al. 1991). Robots have even been designed
that can
recover from perturbations during walking with only open loop methods
of
stabilization (Mombaur, Bock et al. 2005). Consistent with this, people
with
cerebellar agenesis or cerebellar dysfunction (developmental, lesion,
or
alcohol intoxication) can stand and walk with compromised supraspinal
processes
(Titomanlio, Romano et al. 2005, see particularly the associated video
to this
case report), as can those with spinal cord injuries when given partial
hoist
support following intensive multi-year physiotherapy (van Hedel, Wirth
et al.
2005).
However, from an evolutionary
perspective, the existence of such
non-top-down supraspinal internal model based bipedal stabilization
mechanisms
is less relevant than it might appear. This is because such mechanisms
are too
limited in their perturbation resistance to enhance an individual’s
survival
fitness, as would have happened when the evolution of human motor
abilities
occurred during the Pleistocene. People who are “drunk”, for example,
maybe
able to stand and walk but such inebriated individuals lack exact
temporal
organization in their movements, and as a result, they are, in many
respects
disabled, as they easily fall, or are limited in what they can do in a
stable
manner. Modern human locomotion, moreover, is much easier in terms of
maintaining upright balance than it was in Pleistocene times: the
modern
walking environment is mostly paved, flat, often carpeted, and kept
clear of
footing hazards by health and safety legal requirements to minimize
accidents.
In contrast, the Pleistocene environment was rough and full of
unexpected
footing hazards that could potentially trip or slip up walkers and
runners. The
concern in this review is exclusively with the processes that enable
the
exquisite and highly robust balance shown by human adults when engaged
in
evolutionarily exacting upper body biped skills (such as accurate
throwing), or
challenging circumstances (avoiding trips and slips where there are
footing
hazards as when rough ground running), as only these are relevant to
the past
selection of human specific bipedal motor functions.
Summary of
human bipedality and internal
models
This brief discussion of human
bipedality provides preliminary evidence
to suggest that like dexterity, its uniqueness derives from
anticipatory timed
motor stabilization based upon internal models. Another source of
evidence is
that neuroanatomically, like dexterity, lesion and neuroimaging shows
that it
depends upon the cerebello-cerebral cortex circuits (Morton and Bastian
2004;
Ioffe, Chernikova et al. 2007; Jacobs and Horak 2007; Maki and McIlroy
2007) including
prefrontal ones (Suzuki, Miyai et al. 2004; Slobounov, Wu et al. 2006)
that
underlie such internal models.
References
Abitbol, A. M. (1988). "Effect posture and
locomotion on energy
expenditure." American Journal of Physical Anthropology
77:
191-199.
Alexander, R. M. (1992). "A model of bipedal
locomotion on
compliant legs." Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci
338(1284):
189-98.
Alexander, R. M. (2004). "Bipedal animals, and
their
differences from humans." J Anat 204(5): 321-30.
Assaiante, C. and B. Amblard (1995). "An
ontogenetic model for
the sensorimotor organization of balance control in humans." Hum
Mov
Sci 14: 13-43.
Belen'kii, V. E., V. S. Gurfinkel, et al.
(1967). "Elements of
control of voluntary movements." Biophysics 12(1):
154-160.
Berillon, G. (2004). In what manner did they
walk on two legs? From
biped to strider. D. J. Meldrum and C. E. Hilton: 85–100.
Borelli, G. A. (1989/1680). On the
movement of animals.
Bouisset, S. and M. Zattara (1987).
"Biomechanical study of the
programming of anticipatory postural adjustments associated with
voluntary
movement." J Biomech 20(8): 735-42.
Bramble, D. M. and D. E. Lieberman (2004).
"Endurance running
and the evolution of Homo." Nature 432(7015):
345-52.
Cavagna, G. A., H. Thys, et al. (1976). "The
sources of
external work in level walking and running." J Physiol
262(3):
639-57.
Cheng, Y. W., O. J. Tzeng, et al. (2005).
"Modulation of spinal
excitability during observation of bipedal locomotion." Neuroreport
16(15): 1711-4.
Collins, S., A. Ruina, et al. (2005).
"Efficient bipedal robots
based on passive-dynamic walkers." Science
307(5712): 1082-5.
Cordo, P. J. and L. M. Nashner (1982).
"Properties of postural
adjustments associated with rapid arm movements." J
Neurophysiol
47(2): 287-302.
Crompton, R. H., L. Yu, et al. (1998). "The
mechanical
effectiveness of erect and "bent-hip, bent-knee" bipedal walking in
Australopithecus afarensis." J Hum Evol 35(1):
55-74.
de Hoon, E. W., J. H. Allum, et al. (2003).
"Quantitative
assessment of the stops walking while talking test in the elderly." Archives
of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation 84: 838-842.
Desmurget, M. and S. Grafton (2000). "Forward
modeling allows
feedback control for fast reaching movements." Trends in
Cognitive
Sciences 4: 423-431.
Ebersbach, G., M. R. Dimitrijevic, et al.
(1995). "Influence of
concurrent tasks on gait: A dual-task approach." Perceptual
and Motor
Skills 61: 107-113.
Fallis, G. T. (1888). Walking toy: 1-4.
French, M. J. and W. B. Widden (2000). "The
spring-and-lever
balancing mechanism, Geoge Carwardine and the Anglepoise lamp." Proceedings
of Instrument and Mechanical Engineers 214C: 501-508.
Gebo, D. L. and G. T. Schwartz (2006). "Foot
bones from Omo:
implications for hominid evolution." American Journal of
Physical
Anthropology 129(4): 499-511.
Gelfand,
Gregory, W. K. (1912). "Notes on the principles
of quadrupedal
locomotion and on the mechanism of the limbs in hoofed animals." Annals
of the
Gurfinkel, V. S., Y. M. Kots, et al. (1971).
The compensation of
respiratory disturbances of the erect posture of man as an example of
the
organization of interarticular interaction. Models of the
structural-functional organization of certain biological systems.
I. M.
Gelfand, V. S. Gurfinkel, S. V. Fomin and M. L. Tsetlin.
Hauer, R., M. Pfisterer, et al. (2003).
"Cognitive impairment
decreases postural control during dual tasks in geriatric patients with
a
history of severe falls." Journal of the American Geriatrics
Society
51: 1638-1644.
He, J., W. S. Levine, et al. (1991). "Feeback
gains for
correcting small perturbations to standing posture." IEEE
Transactions
on Automated control 36: 322-332.
Hirasakia, E.,
Hirschfeld, H. and H. Forssberg (1992).
"Development of
anticipatory postural adjustments during locomotion in children." Journal
of Neurophysiology 68: 542-550.
Hodges, P. W., V. S. Gurfinkel, et al. (2002).
"Coexistence of
stability and mobility in postural control: evidence from postural
compensation
for respiration." Exp Brain Res 144(3): 293-302.
Hsiao, E. T. and S. N. Robinovitch (1998).
"Common protective
movements govern unexpected falls from standing height." Journal
of
Biomechanics 31: 1-9.
Hughey, L. K. and J. Fung (2005). "Postural
responses triggered
by multidirectional leg lifts and surface tilts." Experimental
Brain
Research 163: 152-166.
Ioffe, M. E., L. A. Chernikova, et al. (2007).
"Role of
cerebellum in learning postural tasks." Cerebellum
6: 87-94.
Jacobs, J. V. and F. B. Horak (2007). "Cortical
control of
postural responses." J Neural Transm 114(10):
1339-48.
Latimer, B. (1991). Locomotor adaptations in Australopithecus afarenis: The issue of
bipedality. Origine(s)
de la bipedie chez les hominides. Y. Coppens and B. Senut.
Latimer, B. (2005). "The perils of being
bipedal." Ann
Biomed Eng 33(1): 3-6.
Lieberman, D. E., D. A. Raichlen, et al.
(2006). "The human
gluteus maximus and its role in running." J Exp Biol
209(Pt 11):
2143-55.
Lotz, J. C. and W. C. Hayes (1990). "The use of
quantitative
computed tomography to estimate risk of fracture of the hip from
falls." J
Bone Joint Surg Am 72(5): 689-700.
Lovejoy, C. O. (2006). "The natural history of
human gait and
posture. Part 3. The knee." Gait Posture.
Lundin-Olsson, L., L. Nyberg, et al. (1997).
"'Stops walking
when talking' as a predictor of falls in elderly people." Lancet
349: 617.
MacKinnon, C. D., D. Bissig, et al. (2007).
"Preparation of
anticipatory postural adjustments prior to stepping." Journal
of
Neurophysiology 97: 4368-4379.
Maki, B. E. and W. E. McIlroy (2007).
"Cognitive demands and
cortical control of human balance-recovery reactions." J
Neural Transm
114(10): 1279-96.
Marigold, D. S., A. J. Bethune, et al. (2003).
"Role of the
unperturbed limb and arms in the reactive recovery response to an
unexpected
slip during locomotion." Journal of Neurophysiology
89(1727-1737).
Massion, J. (1992). "Movement, posture and
equilibrium: interaction
and coordination." Progress in Neurobiology 38:
35-56.
Mombaur, K. D., H. G. Bock, et al. (2005).
Open-loop stable control
of periodic multibody systems. Modeling, simulation and
optimization of
complex processes. H. G. Bock, E. Kostina, H. X. Phu and R.
Rannacher.
Morasso, P. G. and M. Schieppati (1999). "Can
muscle stiffness
alone stabilize upright standing?" Journal of Neurophysiology
83:
1622-1626.
Morton, S. M. and A. J. Bastian (2004).
"Cerebellar control of
balance and locomotion." Neuroscientist 10(3):
247-59.
Nigg, B. M., H. A. Bahlsen, et al. (1987). "The
influence of
running velocity and midsole hardness on external impact forces in
heeel-toe
running." Journal of Biomechanics 20: 951-959.
Ohman, J. C., T. J. Krochta, et al. (1997).
"Cortical bone
distribution in the femoral neck of hominoids: Implications for the
locomotion
of Australopithecus afarensis." American
Journal of Physical Anthropology 104: 117-131.
Okada, M. (1985). Primate bipedal walking:
Comparative kinematics. Primate
morphophysiology, locomotor analyses and human bipedalism. S.
Kondo.
Pai, Y. C., F. Yang, et al. (2006). "Mechanisms
of limb
collapse following a slip among young and older adults." J
Biomech
39(12): 2194-204.
Schmitt, D. (2003). "Insights into the
evolution of human
bipedalism from experimental studies of humans and other primates." J
Exp Biol 206(Pt 9): 1437-48.
Slobounov, S., R. Tutwiler, et al. (2000).
"Human oscillatory
brain activity within gamma band (30-50 Hz) induced by visual
recognition of
non-stable postures." Brain Res Cogn Brain Res
9(2): 177-92.
Slobounov, S., T. Wu, et al. (2006). "Neural
underpinning of
postural responses to visual field motion." Biological
Psychiatry
72: 188-197.
Sockol, M. D., D. A. Raichlen, et al. (2007).
"Chimpanzee
locomotor energetics and the origin of human bipedalism." Proceedings
of the
Stern, J. T. (2000). "Climbing to the top: a
personal memoir of
Australopithecus afarensis." Evolutionary
Anthropology 9: 113-133.
Susman, R. L., J. T. Stern, Jr., et al. (1984).
"Arboreality
and bipedality in the Hadar hominids." Folia Primatologica
43:
113-156.
Suzuki, M.,
Tardieu, C. (1998). "Short adolescence in early
hominids:
Infantile and adolescent growth of the human femur." American
Journal
of Physical Anthropology 107: 163-178.
Thomas, D. P. and R. J. Whitney (1959).
"Postural movements
during normal standing in man." Journal of Anatomy
93: 524-539.
Thorstensson, A., J. Nilsson, et al. (1984).
"Trunk movements
in human locomotion." Acta Physiologica Scandinavica
121: 9-22.
Titomanlio, L., A. Romano, et al. (2005).
"Cerebellar agenesis."
Neurology 64(6): E21.
van Hedel, H. J. A., B. Wirth, et al. (2005).
"Limits of
locomotor ability in subjects with a spinal cord injury." Spinal
Cord
43: 593-603.
VanderVelde, T. J., M. H. Woollacott, et al.
(2005). "Selective
utilization of spatial working memory resources during stance posture."
Neuroreport
16(7): 773-7.
Weber, W. and E. Weber (1992/1825). Mechanics
of the human
walking apparatus.
Winter, D. A. (1991). The
biomechanics and motor control of human
gait.
Winter, D. A., A. E. Patla, et al. (1998).
"Stiffness control
of balance in quiet standing." J Neurophysiol
80(3): 1211-21.
Witte, H., H. Preuschoft, et al. (1991). "Human
body
proportions explained on the basis of biomechanical principles." Zeitschrift
fur Morphologie und Anthropologie 78: 407-423.
Woollacott, M. and A. Shumway-Cook (2002).
"Attention and the
control of posture and gait: a review of an emerging area of research."
Gait
Posture 16(1): 1-14.
Yardley, L., M. Gardner, et al. (2000). "Effect
of articulatory
and mental tasks on postural control." Neuroreport
10: 215-219.
Zettel, J. L., W. E. McIlroy, et al. (2002). "Can stabilizing features of rapid triggered stepping reactions be modulated to meet environmental constraints?" Exp Brain Res 145(3): 297-308.